Saturday, May 25, 2019
Robbers Cave Experiment: A Critical Review
In this paper, the aftermath will be explored in two parts a) An overview of the study would be presented along with a critique of its findings and b) its induction and application to real-world and Asian contexts will be formally discussed. Overview Of study Purpose and design The studs focus was on intercrop relations (Sheriff, AAA)-?specifically cooperation and conflict, where the intricate memberes involved in members attitudes in two or more groups over a period of time were investigated.Sheriff had intended for the experiment to progress as natural as possible, so as to trace the formation, functioning, attitude shifts and proceedsing consequences towards ones own group (in-group), show up-group and their members accurately from scratch. A large-scale experiment, the RACE took place In a sufficiently-isolated (I. E. , without interference or fundamental fundamental interaction with the outside world) field setting in the Robbers Cave State Park, Oklahoma, over a span of mo re than two weeks.Sheriff employed a rigorous social occasion in the selection of 24 participants, all of whom were 1 2-year-old males from middle-class background, Protestant, tit similar educational and socio-cultural background and no prior relationships with one another (Sheriff et al. , ICC). In order for greater experimental control, participants from atypical backgrounds were eliminated-?the boys had to be well-adjusted individuals who cannot come from broken families and were doing well psychologically, physically and academically.The boys were later split into two groups. Staff members participated in the camp under the guise of senior counselors, whose duties were to observe first-hand group interaction behaviors among the boys. Participants were led to believe that they were taking part in a typical pass camp, and that the interaction processes which arose from problem situations were natural products of their existing circumstance or environment. Approach The RACE prog ressed in successive stages (Sheriff et al. 1 95th) 1) Experimental in-group formation, where both groups organise their in-groups and established relations (independently of each other) through activities involving cooperation and commonalty goals 2) Friction phase, where intercrop relations were thoroughly explored through experimentally- reduced through competitive activities that wind frustration for the losing group 3) integrating phase, where both groups are brought together to reduce existing intercrop tensions and encourage harmony to attain subordinate goals (Sheriff et al. 15th) integral to a problem scenario. The goals, which were subsequently introduced, were of significant common appeal and which required both groups equitable cooperation to obtain. If endings Hypotheses of the study, which are not elaborated here, were tested and validated by Sheriff and his team at the conclusion of the RACE. Instead, the following conclusions wasted (Sheriff et al. , IEEE) are m entioned for their relevance to the subsequent sections Intercrop attitudes (e. G. Prejudices) are not merely products of individual personalities or frustrations brought to the situation. In-group solidarity heightened in the face of (real or imagined) competitive and reciprocally-frustrating activities, where outgrip were unfavorable stereotyped. Rather, interaction produced when operative toward common subordinate goals served well to improve inter-group relations and cooperation. A critique To begin, the ARC has been recognized for its high ecological validity (Jackson, 1993).However, it does not explain the process by which subordinate goals reduce inter-group hostility. It also does not fully acknowledge third party influences (e. G. , bystander effect of camp counselors). In respond to this, Jackson (1993) proposed that further theories be advanced. Interestingly, research by Teasel and Turner (1986) (as cited in Brewer, 1975) subsequently challenged the ARC with its social identity theory, noting that overt competition is not always necessary to produce intercrop inflict, and that competition is not always be a bad thing (Valentine, 2010).Beyond these and ethical considerations, the RACE also had many other limitations Its findings cannot be reliably generalize to the wider population due to its homogeneous and gender-bias (Brewer, 1975) sample-?similar studies later conducted in Russia, Lebanon and ELK produced differing results (Determent & Spencer, 1983). It devourms that cultural difference IS an essential variable (Kim & Meyers, 2012) that the RACE had overlooked. The age of the participants may guide also influenced-?cognitively or behaviorally-?how he conflict or cooperation (I. . , interactions) played out. Furthermore, the presenting conflict situations had been staged and variables carefully experimentally-controlled for, which produced UN-realistic outcomes simplistic for real-world generalization. To a lesser extent, psychologists have pointed out that realistic intercrop conflicts were heavily determined by the degree of group identity and loyalty, and that if the two groups had failed in achieving the subordinate goals, the conflict could have exacerbated (e. . , blaming other party) instead of see relief (Brook, 2006). Application of incepts In consideration of the above, while the RACE is a landmark experiment that undoubtedly has its uses in theory (e. G. Generating future research Brewer, 1975), cover version applicability unto real-world contexts would be an erroneous step to take. The ARC suggests that when resources are scarce, people should be especially in-group-biased (Campbell, 1965).Indeed, this phenomenon surpasses time and space. Examples are when the anti-Muslim riots broke out in agency (AY Swashbuckler, 201 3) and negative stereotypes for the Muslim were perpetuated by rioting monks rallied fellow Buddhists to make cuisines with our own people? and when the Nazi regime in the 1 sass propagate d Aryan propaganda and crush the entire Jewish race for causing Germanys economic problems.Also, we can agree with Sheriff that inter- and intra-group attitudes are not mere extrapolations of individuals and their habits, for social-psychological phenomenon such as grouping (Smith & Mann, 1 992) imply the battlefront of hidden and complex processes behind group dynamics. Next, subordinate goals may not necessarily improve intercrop relations, as Sheriff et al. Claimed (1 IEEE). As mentioned, allure to attain these goals can result in mutual blaming frustration and shaming.A good example would be global warming, a trans-boundary issue experienced by all countries. While the common brat is sufficiently real and joint problem-solving should be assumed, countries instead are not able to resolve the issue-?intercrop hostilities (e. G. China with the IIS) heightened as the competition (e. G. , for economic primacy) is too overwhelming. These suggests that interaction and intercrop rela tions are highly dynamic and susceptible to fluctuation. Lastly, let us not forget the instrumentality of ultra difference factors on group relations.The RACE was conducted five decades ago using a racially-homogeneous sample. Modern globalizes societies have become smaller and more heterogeneous with technological advancements. Moreover, many parts of Asiaespecially Southeast-Asia-?see highly-mixed communities comprising dozens of racial/religious groups, each with their unique sub-culture. As such, diversity of the wider population has to be considered and reflected in their appropriate context and in light Of todays fast-changing world.
Subscribe to:
Post Comments (Atom)
No comments:
Post a Comment
Note: Only a member of this blog may post a comment.